La naturaleza del lenguaje: un reto y una propuesta
Conteúdo do artigo principal
Resumo
Detalhes do artigo
Commons Attribution License, CCAL). Com esta licença, os autores conservam o direito de propriedade sobre artigos mas permitem que qualquer pessoa façam download e distribuam os artigos publicados na RACC sem necessidade da permissão do autor ou editor. Uma última condição é que sempre, e em todos os casos, o autor e a fonte original de publicação (p.e., RACC) sejam citados. Esta licença foi desenvolvido para facilitar o acesso aberto, gratuito e livre a trabalhos originais do arte e ciência.
Como Citar
Referências
Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The social nature of words and word learning. En R. M. Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek (eds.), (2000). Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘‘theory of mind’’? Cognition, 21, 37-46.
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Burge, T. (1973). Reference and proper names. Journal of Philosophy. 70(14), 426-39.
Clapp, L. J. (2012). Is even thought compositional? Philosophical Studies, 157, 299-322.
Diesendruck, G. (2005). The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: An empirical examination. Developmental
Psychology, 41, 451-63.
Diesendruck, G. (2007). Mechanisms of word learning. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz (eds.), (2007). Blackwell Handbook of Language Development. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Donnellan, K. (1972). Names and identifying descriptions. In G. Harman, & D. Davidson, (eds.), Semantics for Natural Language. Dordrecht-Holland: D. Reidel.
Ezcurdia, M. (1995). Modos de presentación y modos de determinación. Crítica. Revista Hispanoamericana de Filosofía, XXVII, 80, 57-96.
Ezcurdia, M. (2004). Pragmatic attitudes and semantic competence. Crítica. Revista Hispanoamericana de Filosofía, XXXVI, 108, 55-82.
Frege, G. (1892). On sense and nominatum. In A. P. Martinich (ed.), (2002). The Philosophy of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Friedman, O., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). The conceptual underpinnings of pretense: Pretending is not ‘behaving as if’. Cognition, 105, 103-24.
Fodor, J. A. (2001). Language, thought and compositionality. Mind & Language, 16, 1-15.
García-Ramírez, E. (2011). A cognitive theory of empty names, Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2, 785-807.
Gerken, L. A. (2007). Acquiring linguistic structure. In E. Hoff, & M. Shatz, (2007). Blackwell Handbook of Language Development. Oxford: Blackwell.
Geurts, B. (1998). The mechanisms of denial. Language 74(2), 274-307.
Gleitman, L. R., Cassidy, K., Nappa, R., Papafragou, A., & Trueswell, J. C. (2005). Hard words, Language Learning and Development, 1, 23-64.
Gould, S. J., & Vrba, E. S. (1982). Exaptation, a missing term in the science of form Paleobiology, 8(1), 4-15.
Grice, H. P. (1989). Logic and Conversation. In H. P. Grice, 1989: Studies in the Way of Words. Cambridge, M. A: Harvard University Press.
Hall, G. (1996). Preschooler’s default assumptions about word meaning: Proper names designate unique individuals. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 177-86.
Hall, G., & Graham, S. (1999). Lexical form class information guides word-to-object mapping in preschoolers, Child Development, 70(1), 78-91.
Hoff, E., & Shatz, M. (eds.), (2007). Blackwell Handbook of Language Development. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Jackson, F. (1998). Reference and description revisited, Philosophical Perspectives, 12, Language, Mind, and Ontology, 201-218.
Kaplan, D. (1989). Demonstratives. In J. Almog, et. al., (eds.), (1989). Themes From Kaplan. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kripke, S. (1979). A puzzle about belief. In A. Margalit (ed.), (1979). Meaning and Use. Dordrecht: Synthese.
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of ‘‘theory of mind’’. Psychological Revie, 94, 412-26.
Leslie, A. M. (1994). Pretending and believing: Issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition, 50, 211-38.
Leslie, A. M. (2002). Pretense and representation revisited. In N. L. Stein, P. J. Bauer, M., Rabinowitz, (eds.), (2002). Representation, Memory, and Development: Essays in Honor of Jean Mandler. Mahwah, N. J: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Lewis, D. (1970). General semantics. Synthese, 1(2), 18-67.
Lewis, D. (1975). Languages and language. In D. Lewis 1983: Philosophical Papers, Vol. 1. USA: Oxford University Press.
Lewis, D. (1986). On The Plurality of Worlds. Oxford: Blackwell.
Maiden, M., Smith, J. C., & Ledgeway, A. (2011). The Cambridge History of Romance Languages. Volumen 1 y 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Maiden, M. (2011b). Morphonological Innovation. In Maiden, Smith y Ledgeway (eds.), (2011). The Cambridge History of Romance Languages. Volumen 1 y 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Markman, E. M. (1991). The whole object, taxonomic, and mutual exclusivity assumptions as initial constraints on word meaning. In
S.A. Gelman, & J. P. Byrnes, (eds.), (1991). Perspectives on Language and Thought: Interrrelations in Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Markman, E. M. (1992). Constraints on word learning: Speculations about their nature, origins, and domain specificity. In M. R. Gunnar & M. Maratsos (eds.). Modularity and constrains in language and cognition. Hillsdale, N. J: Erlbaum.
Montague, R. (1974). Formal Philosophy: Selected Papers of Richard Montague. R. Thomasson (ed.). New Haven: Yale University Press.
Nunberg, G. (1979). The non-uniqueness of semantic solutions: Polysemy. In Linguistics and Philosophy 3, 143-84.
Nunberg, G. (1995). Transfers of meaning. In Journal of Semantics 12(2), 109-32.
Nunberg, G. (2004). The pragmatics of deferred interpretation. In L. Horn (ed) 2004: The Handbook of Pragmatics. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109-178. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Perner, J., Rendl, B., & Garnham, A. (2007). Objects of desire, thought, and reality: Problems of anchoring discourse referents in development. Mind & Language, 22(5), 475-513.
Perry, J. (1979). The case of the essential indexical. Nous, 13, 3-21.
Putnam, H. (1975). The meaning of ‘meaning’. In H. Putnam, 1975: Mind, Language and Reality: Philosophical Papers. Vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Quine, W. V. O. (1960). Word and Object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Quine, W. V. O. (1961). Reference and modality. In W. V. O., Quine, (1961). From a Logical Point of View. New York: Harper and Row.
Salmon, N. (1986). Frege’s Puzzle. Ridgeview Publishing.
Shatz, M. (1987). Bootstrapping operations in child language. In K. E. Nelson y A. van Kleeck (eds.), (1987). Children’s Language, Vol. 6, Hilsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Shatz, M. (1994). A Toddler’s Life. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Shatz, M. (2007a). On the development of the field of language development. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz, (eds.), (2007). Blackwell Handbook of Language Development. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Shatz, M. (2007b). Revisiting a Toddler’s Life for the toddler years: Conversational participation as a tool for learning across knowledge domains. In C.A. Brownwell & C.B. Kopp (eds.), (2007). Socioemotional Development in the Toddler Years. Guilford Press.
Shatz, M. (2008). Language as a consequence and an enabler of the exercise of higher-order relational capabilities: Evidence from toddlers. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 31, 145-146.
Smith, J. C. (2011). Change and continuity in form-function relationships. In Maiden, Smith & Ledgeway (eds.), (2011). The Cambridge History of Romance Languages. Volumen 1 y 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Soames, S. (2002). Beyond Rigidity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Spelke, E. (2003). What makes us smart? Core Knowledge and Natural Language. In D. Gentner & S. Golding-Meadow, (eds.), (2003). Language in Mind: Advances in the Investigation of Language and Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Stalnaker, R. (1987). Semantics for belief. In R., Stalnaker (1999). Context and Content. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stanley, J. (2005). Semantics in context. In G. Preyer & G. Peter, (eds.), (2005). Contextualism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Strawson, P. F. (1950). On Referring. Mind. 59(235), 320-344.
Szabó, Z. (2012). The case for Compositionality. In Werning, Hinzen & Machery (eds.), (2012). The Oxford Handbook of Compositionality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Atypical language development: Autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz
(eds.), (2007). Blackwell Handbook of Language Development. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Travis, C. (1997). Pragmatics. In B. Hale & C. Wright. (eds.), (1997). A Companion to the Philosophy of Language. Oxford: Blackwell.
Wellman, H. M., & Miller, J. G. (2008). Including deontic reasoning as fundamental to theory of mind, Human Development, 51, 105-35.