Affective Simon Effect: A study in children and adolescents Affective Simon Effect

Contenido principal del artículo

Eliana Zamora
Isabel Introzzi
Macarena del Valle
Yesica Aydmune
María Richard's

Resumen

In this study, a modified variant of the Affective Simon Task (De Houwer & Eelen, 1998) was used in children and adolescents. Thus, we designed a task in which the participants had to respond choosing a happy or sad face depending on whether the figure presented on the screen was an object or an animal, ignoring its affective valence (positive or negative). Our results showed that the Affective Simon effect was produced only in the children group. Likewise, it was found that performance improved with age, which indicates an improvement of the processes involved in the resolution of the Affective Simon effect. The results of this study are a first exploratory approach to the investigation of the Affective Simon effect in children and young populations.

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.

Detalles del artículo

Cómo citar
Zamora, E., Introzzi, I., del Valle, M., Aydmune, Y., & Richard’s, M. (2024). Affective Simon Effect: A study in children and adolescents: Affective Simon Effect. Revista Argentina De Ciencias Del Comportamiento, 16(2), 37–50. https://doi.org/10.32348/1852.4206.v16.n2.37662
Sección
Artículos Originales

Citas

American Psychological Association (2010). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist. http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/principles.pdf

Argentinean Social Debt Observatory (s.f.). Página de inicio de la comunidad. Universidad Católica Argentina. https://repositorio.uca.edu.ar/handle/123456789/720

Amso, D., & Casey, B. J. (2009). Beyond what develops when: Neuroimaging may inform how cognition changes with development. En L. S. Liben (Ed.), Current directions in developmental psychology (pp. 85-94). Pearson.

Amso, D., & Johnson, S. P. (2005). Selection and inhibition in infancy: Evidence from the spatial negative priming paradigm. Cognition, 95(2), B27-B36. ttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.006

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, Aging, and Cognitive Control: Evidence From the Simon Task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290

Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032-1053. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00333

Casey, B. J. (2005) Frontostriatal and frontocerebellar circuitry underlying cognitive control. En U. Mayr, E. Awh, & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Developing Individuality in the Human Brain: A tribute to Michael I (pp. 141–166). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11108-008

Cicchetti, D. V., & Sparrow, S. A. (1981). Developing Criteria for Establishing Interrater Reliability of Specific Items: Applications to Assessment of Adaptive-Behavior. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 86(2), 127–137. https://europepmc.org/article/med/7315877

Cohen, R. J., & Swerdlik, M. E. (2010). Psychological Testing and Assessment: An Introduction to Tests and Measurement (7th edition). McGraw-Hill Companies.

Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (2006). Lineamientos para el comportamiento ético en las ciencias sociales y humanidades. Resolución 2857/06. Buenos Aires, Argentina: Ministerio de Educación, Ciencia y Tecnología. http://www.conicet.gov.ar/wp-content/uploads/RD-20061211-2857.pdf

Constantinidis, C., & Luna, B. (2019). Neural Substrates of Inhibitory Control Maturation in Adolescence. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(9), 604-616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2019.07.004

Cragg, L. (2016). The Development of Stimulus and Response Interference Control in Midchildhood. Developmental Psychology, 52(2), 242-252. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000074

Cragg, L. & Nation, K. (2008). Go or no go? Developmental improvements in the efficiency of response inhibition in mid-childhood. Developmental Science, 11(6), 819-827. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00730.x

Dahl, R. E. (2001). Affect regulation, brain development, and behavioral/ emotional health in adolescence. CNS Spectrums, 6(1), 60-72. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852900022884

Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037-2078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006

De Houwer, J. (2003a). The extrinsic affective Simon task. Experimental Psychology, 50(2), 77-85. https://doi.org/10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.77

De Houwer, J. (2003b). A structural analysis of indirect measures of attitudes. En J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 219-244). Erlbaum.

De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1998). An affective variant of the Simon paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 12(1), 45-62. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999398379772

De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., Baeyens, F., & Hermans, D. (2001). On the generality of the affective Simon effect. Cognition & Emotion, 15(2), 189-206. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930125883

De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., & Eelen, P. (1998). Affective and identity priming with episodically associated stimuli. Cognition & Emotion, 12(2), 145-169. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999398379691

de Jong, P., van den Hout, M., Rietbroek, H., & Huijding, J. (2003). Dissociations between implicit and explicit attitudes toward phobic stimuli. Cognition & Emotion, 17(4), 521-545. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930302305

Draheim, C., Hicks, K. L., & Engle, R. W. (2016). Combining Reaction Time and Accuracy: The Relationship Between Working Memory Capacity and Task Switching as a Case Example. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(1), 133–155. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615596990

Duscherer, K., Holender, D., & Molenaar, E. (2008). Revisiting the affective Simon effect. Cognition & Emotion, 22(2), 193-217. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701339228

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149-1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

Fleiss, J. L., Levin, B. & Paik, M.C. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions (2nd ed.). John Wiley.

Fuentes Melero, L., & García Sevilla, J. (2008). Manual de Psicología de la atención. Una perspectiva neurocientífica. Síntesis.

Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68(1), 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y

Introzzi, I., Aydmune, Y., Zamora, E. V., Vernucci, S., & Ledesma, R. (2019). The development mechanisms of selective attention in child population. CES Psicología, 12(3), 105-118. https://doi.org/10.21615/cesp.12.3.8

Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86(2-3), 199-225. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(94)90003-5

Kornblum, S. (1992). Dimensional overlap and dimensional relevance in stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility. En G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior II, 2 (pp. 743-777). Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Kornblum, S., & Lee, J. W. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 855-875. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.4.855

Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2(2), 174-207. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210959

Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., Hooper, C. J., & Yarger, R. S. (2005). The development of nonverbal working memory and executive control processes in adolescents. Child Development, 76(3), 697-712. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00872.x

Luna, B., Garver, K. E., Urban, T. A., Lazar, N. A., & Sweeney, J. A. (2004). Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Development, 75(5), 1357-1372. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00745.x

Marchewka, A., Żurawski, Ł., Jednoróg, K., & Grabowska, A. (2014). The Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS): Introduction to a novel, standardized, wide-range, high-quality, realistic picture database. Behavior Research Methods, 46(2), 596-610. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0379-1

Mullane, J. C., Corkum, P. V., Klein, R. M., & McLaughlin, E. (2009). Interference control in children with and without ADHD: a systematic review of Flanker and Simon task performance. Child Neuropsychology, 15(4), 321-342. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040802348028

Nweze, T., & Nwani, W. (2020). Contributions of working memory and inhibition to cognitive flexibility in Nigerian adolescents. Developmental Neuropsychology, 45(3), 118-128, https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2020.1765169

Parsons, S. (2019). Splithalf: Robust estimates of split half reliability (R package Version 5) [Computer software]. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5559175.v5

Pauls, F., Macha, T., & Petermann, F. (2013). U-shaped development: an old but unsolved problem. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article e301. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00301

Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy-Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1-2), 8-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017

Pellicano, A., Iani, C., Borghi, A. M., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2010). Simon-like and functional affordance effects with tools: The effects of object perceptual discrimination and object action state. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2190-2201. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.486903

Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 380-391. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380

Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: Data, theory, and application. Taylor & Francis CRC Press.

Richardson, C., Anderson, M., Reid, C. L., & Fox, A. M. (2018). Development of inhibition and switching: A longitudinal study of the maturation of interference suppression and reversal processes during childhood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 34, 92-100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.002

Schimmack, U, & Derryberry, D. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: threat, negativity, or arousal?. Emotion, 5(1), 55-66. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.55

Silvers, J. A., McRae, K., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Gross, J. J., Remy, K. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). Age-related differences in emotional reactivity, regulation, and rejection sensitivity in adolescence. Emotion, 12(6), 1235–1247. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028297

Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300-304. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020586

Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 69-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005

Tipples, J. (2001). A conceptual replication and extension of the Affective Simon Effect. Cognition & Emotion, 15(5), 705-710. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930125941

Vandierendonck, A. (2017). A comparison of methods to combine speed and accuracy measures of performance: A rejoinder on the binning procedure. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 653–673. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0721-5

Vara, A. S., Pang, E. W., Vidal, J., Anagnostou, E., & Taylor, M. J. (2014). Neural mechanisms of inhibitory control continue to mature in adolescence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 129-139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.009

Voß, A., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2003). Estimating the valence of single stimuli: A new variant of the affective Simon task. Experimental Psychology, 50(2), 86-96. https://doi.org/10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.86

Wagenmakers, E. J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., Selker, R., Gronau, Q. F., Dropmann, D., Boutin, B., Meerhoff, F., Knight, P., Raj, A., van Kesteren, E. J., van Doorn, J., Šmíra, M., Epskamp, S., Etz, A., Matzke, D., ... Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 58–76 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1323-7

Wentura, D. (2019). Cognition and emotion: on paradigms and metaphors. Cognition & Emotion, 33(1), 85-93. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1567464

Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3

World Medical Association. (2013). Declaration of Helsinki - Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. https://www.wma.net/policiespost/wma-declaration-of-helsinki-ethical-principles-formedical-research-involving-human-subjects/

Zamora, E. V., Richard´s, M. M., Introzzi, I., Aydmune, Y., Urquijo, S., Guardia Olmos, J., & Marchewka, A. (2020). The Nencki Affective Picture System: A Children-Rated Subset. Trends in Psychology, 28, 477–493. https://doi.org/10.1007/s43076-020-00029-z

Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 354–360. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012. 00246.x

Zelazo, P. D., & Cunningham, W. A. (2007). Executive function: Mechanisms underlying emotion regulation. En J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 135–158). Guilford Press.