El rol mediador de la pobreza en la asociación entre factores ambientales y el desempeño cognitivo de preescolares

Contenido principal del artículo

Carolina Fracchia
María Soledad Segretin
María Julia Hermida
Lucía Prats
Sebastián J. Lipina

Resumen

La asociación entre los factores ambientales y el desempeño cognitivo durante la infancia podría estar mediada por la pertenencia a hogares pobres (i.e., hogares con necesidades básicas insatisfechas o satisfechas). Este estudio exploró tal mediación en preescolares de diferentes contextos socioeconómicos. Para tal fin, se administraron tareas que demandaron atención ejecutiva, memoria de trabajo, control inhibitorio, planificación y razonamiento fluido a 250 niños/as de 4 y 5 años. Los resultados sugirieron que la pobreza medió los efectos de la composición familiar, la salud infantil, los factores de riesgo para la salud, cantidad de niños/as y adultos en el hogar, la edad materna y las actividades de alfabetización sobre la atención ejecutiva, el razonamiento fluido y el control inhibitorio. Estos resultados contribuyen a la comprensión de la relación entre los factores ambientales y el desarrollo cognitivo a través de la identificación de la pobreza como variable mediadora.

Detalles del artículo

Cómo citar
El rol mediador de la pobreza en la asociación entre factores ambientales y el desempeño cognitivo de preescolares. (2020). Revista Argentina De Ciencias Del Comportamiento, 12(2), 24-38. https://doi.org/10.32348/1852.4206.v12.n2.26482
Sección
Artículos Originales

Cómo citar

El rol mediador de la pobreza en la asociación entre factores ambientales y el desempeño cognitivo de preescolares. (2020). Revista Argentina De Ciencias Del Comportamiento, 12(2), 24-38. https://doi.org/10.32348/1852.4206.v12.n2.26482

Referencias

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485-493. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.485

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182.

Bell, M. A., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2007). Biological systems and the development of self-regulation: Integrating behavior, genetics, and psychophysiology. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 28(5), 409-420. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181131fc7

Berch, D. B., Krikorian, R., & Huha, E. M. (1998). The corsi block-tapping task: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain and Cognition, 38(3), 317-338. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.1039

Berg, W. K., & Byrd, D. L. (2002). The Tower of London spatial problem-solving task: Enhancing clinical and research implementation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(5), 586-604. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.5.586.1006

Bergman Nutley, S., Söderqvist, S., Bryde, S., Thorell, L. B., Humphreys, K., & Klingberg, T. (2011). Gains in fluid intelligence after training non‐verbal reasoning in 4‐year‐old children: A controlled, randomized study. Developmental Science, 14(3), 591-601. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01022.x

Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Willoughby, M., Mills‐Koonce, R., Cox, M., Greenberg, M. T., … Fortunato, C. K. (2011). Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Development, 82(6), 1970-1984. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x

Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2016). Poverty, stress, and brain development: New directions for prevention and intervention. Academic Pediatrics, 16(3), S30-S36. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2016.01.010

Blair, C., Ursache, A., Greenberg, M., Vernon-Feagans, L., & The Family Life Project Investigators. (2015). Multiple Aspects of Self-Regulation Uniquely Predict Mathematics but Not Letter–Word Knowledge in the Early Elementary Grades. Developmental Psychology, 51(4), 459-472. doi: 10.1037/a0038813

Boltvinik, J. (1995). Poverty Measurement Methods: An overview. Washington, DC.: UNDP, Social Development & Poverty Elimination Division.

Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371-399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. En R. Vasta (Ed.), Six theories of child development: Revised formulations and current issues (pp. 187-249). London, England: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. The Future of Children, 7(2), 55-71. doi: 10.2307/1602387

Bull, R., & Lee, K. L. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), 36-41. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12059

Campbell, F. A., Ramey, C. T., Pungello, E., Sparling, J., Miller-Johnson, S., & Early Childhood Education. (2002). Young adult outcomes from the Abecedarian Project. Applied Developmental Science, 6(1), 42-57. doi: 10.1207/S1532480XADS0601_05

Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Cruess Anderson, L., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4-13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037-2078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006

Debelak, R., Egle, J., Köstering, L., & Kaller, C. P. (2016). Assessment of planning ability: Psychometric analyses on the unidimensionality and construct validity of the Tower of London Task (TOL-F). Neuropsychology, 30(3), 346-360. doi: 10.1037/neu0000238

D'Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R. (2015). The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 115-142. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135-168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

Dickerson, A., & Popli, G. K. (2016). Persistent poverty and children's cognitive development: evidence from the UK Millennium Cohort Study. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society), 179(2), 535-558. doi: 10.1111/rssa.12128

Duncan, G. J., & Magnuson, K. (2012). Socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning: Moving from correlation to causation. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(3), 377-386. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1176

Duncan, G. J., Magnuson, K., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2017). Moving beyond correlations in assessing the consequences of poverty. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 413-434. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044224

Evans, G. W. (2016). Childhood poverty and adult psychological well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(52), 14949-14952. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1604756114

Fall, C. H., Sachdev, H. S., Osmond, C., Restrepo-Mendez, M. C., Victora, C., Martorell, R., … Richter, L. M. (2015). Association between maternal age at childbirth and child and adult outcomes in the offspring: a prospective study in five low-income and middle-income countries (COHORTS collaboration). The Lancet Global Health, 3(7), e366-e377. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(15)00038-8

Farah, M. J., Betancourt, L., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., … Hurt, H. (2008). Environmental stimulation, parental nurturance and cognitive development in humans. Developmental Science, 11(5), 793-801. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00688.x

Farah, M. J., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., … Hurt, H. (2006). Childhood poverty: specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research, 1110(1), 166-174. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.072

Fergusson, D. M., & Lynskey, M. T. (1993). Maternal

age and cognitive and behavioral outcomes in middle childhood. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 7(1), 77-91. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.1993.tb00604.x

Finegood, E. D., Raver, C. C., DeJoseph, M. L., & Blair, C. (2017). Parenting in poverty: Attention bias and anxiety interact to predict parents’ perceptions of daily parenting hassles. Journal of Family Psychology, 31(1), 51-60. doi: 10.1037/fam0000291

Fracchia, C. S., Giovannetti, F., Gili, J., Lopez-Rosenfeld, M., Hermida, M. J., Prats, L. M., … Lipina, S. J. (2016). Individuality and Self-regulation in Preschoolers. Diskurs Kindheits-und Jugendforschung, 11(4), 457-471. doi: 10.3224/diskurs.v11i4.25604

Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31-60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31

Goux, D., & Maurin, E. (2005). The effect of overcrowded housing on children's performance at school. Journal of Public Economics, 89(5), 797-819. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2004.06.005

Green, C. T., Bunge, S. A., Chiongbian, V. B., Barrow, M., & Ferrer, E. (2017). Fluid reasoning predicts future mathematical performance among children and adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 157, 125-143. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.12.005

Guo, G., & Mullan Harris, K. (2000). The mechanisms mediating the effects of poverty on children’s intellectual development. Demography, 37(4), 431-447. doi: 10.1353/dem.2000.0005

Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 65-73. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003

Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meany, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(9), 651-659. doi: 10.1038/nrn2897

Hackman, D. A., Gallop, R., Evans, G. W., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Socioeconomic status and executive function: developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science, 18(5), 686-702. doi: 10.1111/desc.12246

Hoelscher, D. M., Moag-Stahlberg, A., Ellis, K., Vandewater, E. A., & Malkani, R. (2016). Evaluation of a student participatory, low-intensity program to improve school wellness environment and students’ eating and activity behaviors. The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 13(1), 59. doi: 10.1186/s12966-016-0379-5

Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 174-180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006

Huang, A. S., Klein, D. N., & Leung, H. C. (2016). Load-related brain activation predicts spatial working memory performance in youth aged 9-12 and is associated with executive function at earlier ages. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1-9. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.10.007

Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(19), 6829-6833. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801268105

Johnson, S. B., Riis, J. L., & Noble, K. G. (2016). State of the art review: Poverty and the developing brain. Pediatrics, 137(4), e20153075. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-3075

Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. D. (1990). Kaufman brief intelligence test: KBIT. Circle Pines, MN: AGS, American Guidance Service.

Kegel, C. A., & Bus, A. G. (2014). Evidence for causal relations between executive functions and alphabetic skills based on longitudinal data. Infant and Child Development, 23(1), 22-35. doi: 10.1002/icd.1827

Kishiyama, M. M., Boyce, W. T., Jimenez, A. M., Perry, L. M., & Knight, R. T. (2009). Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(6), 1106-1115. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21101

Lawson, G. M., Hook, C. J., Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., Griffin, J. A., Freund, L. S., & McCardle, P. (2014). Socioeconomic status and neurocognitive development: Executive function. En J. A. Griffin, L. S. Freund, & P. McCardle (Eds.), Executive Function in Preschool Children: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopment, and Translational Research (pp. 1-28). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.

Lengua, L. J., Moran, L., Zalewski, M., Ruberry, E., Kiff, C., & Thompson, S. (2015). Relations of growth in effortful control to family income, cumulative risk, and adjustment in preschool-age children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(4), 705-720. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9941-2

Leventhal, T., & Newman, S. (2010). Housing and child development. Children and Youth Services Review, 32(9), 1165-1174. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.03.008

Liberzon, I., Ma, S. T., Okada, G., Ho, S. S., Swain, J. E., & Evans, G. W. (2015). Childhood poverty and recruitment of adult emotion regulatory neurocircuitry. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(11), 1596-1606. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv045

Lipina, S. J. (2016). Critical considerations about the

use of poverty measures in the study of cognitive development. International Journal of Psychology, 52(3), 241-250. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12282

Lipina, S. J., & Colombo, J. A. (2009). Poverty and brain development during childhood: An approach from Cognitive Psychology and Neuroscience. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/11879-000

Lipina, S. J., Martelli, M. I., Vuelta, B. L., & Colombo, J. A. (2005). Performance on the AnoB task of Argentinean infants from Unsatisfied Basic Needs Homes. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 39(1), 49-60.

Lipina, S. J., Segretin, M. S., Hermida, M. J., Prats, L., Fracchia, C., López-Camelo, J., & Colombo, J. A. (2013). Linking childhood poverty and cognition: Individual and environmental predictors of non-verbal executive control in an Argentine sample. Developmental Science, 16(5), 697-707. doi: 10.1111/desc.12080

Luby, J., Belden, A., Botteron, K., Marrus, N., Harms, M. P., Babb, C., … Barch, D. (2013). The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stress1ful life events. JAMA Pediatrics, 167(12), 1135-1142. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.3139

Luciana, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Assessment of neuropsychological function through use of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery: performance in 4- to 12-year-old children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 22(3), 595-624. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2203_3

Martínez, A. A., & Nicolini, E. A. (2017). The long memory of poverty: The Historical Unsatisfied Basic Needs and the geographic patterns of standards of living in Argentina (and Spain) in the last 100 years. Trabajo presentado en Iberometric VIII: Eight Iberian Cliometrics Workshop, Institute of Advanced Research in Business and Economics (INARBE), Universidad Pública de Navarra, Pamplona.

McClelland, M. M., Ponitz, C. C., Messersmith, E. E., & Tominey, S. (2010). Self‐regulation: The integration of cognition and emotion. En Lerner, R. M. (Ed.), The Handbook of Life‐Span Development (pp. 510-553). doi: 10.1002/9780470880166.hlsd001015

Minujin, A. (1992). Cuesta abajo. Los nuevos pobres: efectos de la crisis en la sociedad argentina. Buenos Aires, Argentina: Editorial Losada.

Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., & Sears, M. R. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693-2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108

Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., McClelland, M. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2016). The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1744-1762. doi: 10.1037/dev0000159

Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual Research Review: On the relations among self‐regulation, self‐control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk‐taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361-383. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675

Noble, K. G., McCandliss, B. D., & Farah, M. J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science, 10(4), 464-480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x

Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1998). The neuroimaging of human brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 763-764. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.763

Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Tang, Y. (2013). Developing self-regulation in early childhood. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 2(3), 107-110. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2013.09.001

Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., & Hecht, S. A. (2010). Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(2), 110-122. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.005

Rao, H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., Korczykowski, M., Avants, B. B., … Detre, J. A. (2010). Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: evidence from brain morphology in humans. NeuroImage, 49(1), 1144-1150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.003

Rhoades, B. L., Greenberg, M. T., Lanza, S. T., & Blair, C. (2011). Demographic and Familial Predictors of Early Executive Function Development: Contribution of a person-centered perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 638-662. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.004

Ronfani, L., Vecchi Brumatti, L., Mariuz, M., Tognin, V., Bin, M., Ferluga, V., … & Barbone, F. (2015). The Complex Interaction between Home Environment, Socioeconomic Status, Maternal IQ and Early Child Neurocognitive Development: A Multivariate Analysis of Data Collected in a Newborn Cohort Study. PLoS ONE, 10(5), e0127052. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127052

Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Posner, M. I. (2008). Executive attention and effortful control: Linking temperament, brain networks, and genes. Child Development Perspectives, 1(1), 2-7. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2007.00002.x

Rubio-Codina, M., Attanasio, O., & Grantham-McGregor, S. (2016). Mediating pathways in the socio-economic gradient of child development: Evidence from children 6–42 months in Bogota.

International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(6), 483-491. doi: 10.1177/0165025415626515

Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Pappert Lercari, L., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029-1040. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012

Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(41), 14931-14936. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506897102

Sameroff, A. J. (1998). Environmental risk factors in infancy. Pediatrics, 102, 1287-1292.

Sarsour, K., Sheridan, M., Jutte, D., Nuru-Jeter, A., Hinshaw, S., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: the roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood. Journal of International Neuropsychology Society, 17(1), 120-132. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001335

Schelble, J. L., Therriault, D. J., & Miller, M. D. (2012). Classifying retrieval strategies as a function of working memory. Memory & Cognition, 40(2), 218-230. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0149-1

Sdoia, S., Di Nocera, F., & Ferlazzo, F. (2019). Memory for positional movements as a component of the visuospatial working memory. Cognitive Processing, 20(3), 363-369. doi: 10.1007/s10339-019-00917-y

Segretin, M. S., Hermida, M. J., Prats, L. M., Fracchia, C. S., Ruetti, E., & Lipina, S. J. (2016). Childhood Poverty and Cognitive Development in Latin America in the 21st Century. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 152, 9-29. doi: 10.1002/cad.20162

Segretin, M. S., Lipina, S. J., Hermida, M. J., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Espy, K. A., & Colombo, J. A. (2014). Predictors of cognitive enhancement after training in a sample of Argentinean preschoolers from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds. Frontiers in Developmental Psychology, 13, A205. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00205

Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transcripts of the Royal Society of London, 298(1089), 199-209. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1982.0082

Sharkins, K. A., Leger, S. E., & Ernest, J. M. (2016). Examining Effects of Poverty, Maternal Depression, and Children’s Self-Regulation Abilities on the Development of Language and Cognition in Early Childhood: An Early Head Start Perspective. Early Childhood Education Journal, 45(4), 493-498. doi: 10.1007/s10643-016-0787-9

Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657-1661. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5408.1657

Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., & Neville, H. (2009). Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: an event-related brain potential study. Developmental Science, 12(4), 634-646. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00807.x

Sulik, M. J., Blair, C., Mills-Koonce, R., Berry, D., Greenberg, M., & The Family Life Project Investigators. (2015). Early Parenting and the Development of Externalizing Behavior Problems: Longitudinal Mediation through Children’s Executive Function. Child Development, 86(5), 1588-1603. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12386

Ursache, A., Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). The promotion of self-regulation as a means of enhancing school readiness and early achievement in children at risk for school failure. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 122-128. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00209.x

Ursache, A., & Noble, K. G. (2016). Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: Multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology, 53(1), 71-82. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12547

Ursache, A., Noble, K. G., & Blair, C. (2015). Socioeconomic status, subjective social status, and perceived stress: Associations with stress physiology and executive functioning. Behavioral Medicine, 41(3), 145-154. doi: 10.1080/08964289.2015.1024604

Vernon-Feagans, L., Willoughby, M., & Garrett-Peters, P. (2016). Predictors of Behavioral Regulation in Kindergarten: Household Chaos, Parenting, and Early Executive Functions. Developmental Psychology, 52(3), 430-441. doi: 10.1037/dev0000087

Weiland, C., & Yoshikawa, H. (2013). Impacts of a prekindergarten program on children´s mathematics, language, literacy, executive functions, and emotional skills. Child Development, 84(6), 2112-2130. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12099

Weitzman, M. (2007). Low income and its impact on psychosocial child development. En R. E. Tremblay, M. Boivin, & R. De V. Peters (Eds.), Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development (pp. 1-8). Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development and Strategic Knowledge Cluster on Early Child Development.

Yoshikawa, H., Aber, J. L., & Beardslee, W. R. (2012), The effects of poverty on the mental, emotional, and behavioral health of children and youth. American Psychologist, 67(4), 272-284. doi: 10.1037/a0028015

Zauche, L. H., Thul, T. A., Mahoney, A. E. D., & Stapel-Wax, J. L. (2016). Influence of language nutrition

on children’s language and cognitive development: An integrated review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36(3), 318-333. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.01.015

Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 197-206. doi: 10.1086/651257