Evidence of ABC instrumental renewal formerly eliminated by extinction and omission procedures

Main Article Content

Rodolfo Bernal-Gamboa
Javier Nieto

Abstract

One experiment used a free operant procedure with rats to evaluate renewal of a response previously eliminated by using omission training or by using an extinction treatment. All rats were trained to press a lever for food in context A. Then, the operant responses were eliminated in a second context. In this phase, half of the subjects received an omission training (i. e., delivery of food for withholding responding), while the other half received an extinction procedure. Finally, a within-subject ABC renewal test was conducted. The results demonstrated that all rats showed higher levels of responding when they were tested outside extinction context, regardless the procedure used during the elimination phase.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Article Details

How to Cite
Bernal-Gamboa, R., & Nieto, J. (2018). Evidence of ABC instrumental renewal formerly eliminated by extinction and omission procedures. Argentinean Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 10(2), 11–18. https://doi.org/10.32348/1852.4206.v10.n2.19164
Section
Original Articles

References

American Psychological Association. (2010). 2010 amendments to the 2002 “Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct.” American Psychologist, 65(5), 493. doi: 10.1037/a0020168.

Bernal-Gamboa, R., Carrasco-López, M., & Nieto, J. (2014). Contrasting ABA, AAB and ABC renewal in a free operant procedure. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 17, E67. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2014.68.

Bernal-Gamboa, R., Nieto, J., & Uengoer, M. (2017). Effects of extinction in multiple contexts on renewal of instrumental responses. Behavioral Processes, 142, 64-69. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.06.003

Bouton, M. E. (1997). Signals for whether versus when an event will occur. En M. E. Bouton & M. S. Fanselow (Eds.), Learning, motivation, and cognition: The functional behaviorism of Robert C. Bolles (pp. 385-409). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11(5), 485-494. doi: 10.1101/lm.78804

Bouton, M. E. (2014). Why behavior change is difficult to sustain. Preventive Medicine, 68, 29-36. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.06.010

Bouton, M. E., Todd, T. P., Vurbic, D. & Winterbauer, N. E. (2011). Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior. Learning and

Behavior, 39(1), 57-67. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0018-6

Bouton, M. E., & Woods, A. M. (2008). Extinction: Behavioral mechanisms and their implications. En J. H. Byrne, D. Sweatt, R.

Menzel, H. Eichenbaum, & H. Roediger (Eds.), Learning and Memory: A comprehensive reference. Vol. 1, Learning Theory and Behaviour (pp 151-171). Oxford: Elsevier.

Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Crombag, H. S., Bossert, J. M., Koya, E., & Shaham, Y. (2008). Context-induced relapse to drug seeking; a review. Philosophical

Transactions of Royal Society B, 363(1507), 3233-3243. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0090

Crombag, H. S., & Shaham, Y. (2002). Renewal of drug seeking by contextual cues after prolonged extinction in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116(1), 169–173. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.116.1.169

Diario Oficial de la Federación (2001). Norma Oficial Mexicana NOM-062-ZOO-1999: Especificaciones técnicas para la Producción, Cuidado y Uso de los Animales de Laboratorio. Recuperado de: http://www.ibt.unam.mx/computo/pdfs/bioterio.NOM-062.pdf

Dickinson, A., & Charnock, D. J. (1985). Contingency effects with maintained instrumental reinforcement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37(4b), 397–416. doi: 10.1080/14640748508401177

González-Montiel, J. C., Bernal-Gamboa, R., & Nieto, J. (2018). Recuperación de la respuesta de búsqueda de alcohol inducida por el cambio de contexto. Manuscrito en revisión.

Kelley, M. E., Liddon, C. J., Ribeiro, A., Greif, A. E., & Podlesnik, C. A. (2015). Basic and translational evaluation of renewal of operant responding. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 48(2), 390-401. doi: 10.1002/jaba.209

Marchant, N. J., Li, X., & Shaham, Y. (2013). Recent developments in animal models of drug relapse. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(4), 675-683. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.003

Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (2006). Rates and predictors of relapse after natural and treated remission from alcohol use disorders. Addiction, 101(2), 212-222. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01310.x

Moss, J. D., Ruthven A. V. J., Hawkins, W. M., & Topping, J. S. (1983). The relative effects of three response-elimination procedures in severely /profound y and trainable retarded individuals. The Psychological Record, 33(2), 231-236. doi: 10.1007/BF03394840

Nakajima, S., Tanaka, S., Urushihara, K., & Imada, H. (2000). Renewal of extinguished lever-press responses upon return to the training context. Learning and Motivation, 31(4), 416-431. doi: 10.1006/lmot.2000.1064

Nakajima, S., Urushihara, K., & Masaki, T. (2002). Renewal of operant performance formerly eliminated by omission or noncontingency training upon return to the acquisition context. Learning and Motivation, 33(4), 510-525. doi: 10.1016/S0023-

(02)00009-7

Nieto, J., Uengoer, M., & Bernal-Gamboa, R. (2017). A reminder of extinction reduces relapse in an animal model of voluntary behavior. Learning & Memory, 24(2), 76–80. doi: 10.1101/lm.044495.116

Poling, A., & Ryan, C. (1982). Differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedules: Therapeutic applications. Behavior Modification, 6(1), 3-21. doi: 10.1177/01454455820061001

Rescorla, R. A. (1993). Inhibitory associations between S and R in extinction. Animal Learning and Behavior, 21(4), 327–336. doi: 10.3758/BF03197998

Rosas, J. M., & Callejas-Aguilera, J. E. (2006). Context Switch Effects on Acquisition and Extinction in Human Predictive Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(3), 461-474. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.461.

Rosas, J. M., Callejas-Aguilera, J. E., Ramos-Álvarez, M. M., & Abad, M. J. F. (2006). Revision of retrieval theory of forgetting: what does make information context-specific? International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 6(2), 147-166.

Sociedad Mexicana de Psicología. (2009). Código ético del psicólogo. México: Editorial Trillas.

Todd, T. P. (2013). Mechanisms of renewal after the extinction of instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 39(3), 193-207. doi: 10.1037/a0032236

Todd, T. P., Winterbauer, N. E., & Bouton, M. E. (2012). Effects of the amount of acquisition and contextual generalization on the renewal of instrumental behavior after extinction. Learning and Behavior, 40(2), 145-157. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0051-5

Topping, J. S., Thompson, H. J., & Barrios, B. A. (1976). Comparison of omission training and extinction training in mentally retarded individuals. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 8(3), 211–214. doi: 10.3758/BF03335129

Uhl, C. N. (1973). Eliminating behavior with omission and extinction after varying amounts of training. Animal Learning & Behavior, 1(3), 237–240. doi: 10.3758/BF03199082

Uhl, C. N., & Garcia, E. E. (1969). Comparison of omission with extinction in response elimination in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 69(3), 554–562. doi: 10.1037/h0028243

Vollmer, T. R., & Iwata, B. A. (1992). Differential reinforcement as treatment for behavior disorders: procedural and functional variations. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 13(4), 393–417. doi: 10.1016/0891-4222(92)90013-V

Willcocks, A. L., & McNally, G. P. (2014). An extinction retrieval cue attenuates renewal but not reacquisition of alcohol seeking. Behavioral Neuroscience, 128(1), 83-91. doi: 10.1037/a0035595

Zironi, I., Burattini, C., Aircardi, G., & Janak, P. H. (2006). Context is a trigger for relapse to alcohol. Behavioural Brain Research, 167(1), 150–155. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.09.007