Aprendizajes sensoriales tempranos y su relación con las conductas de apego
Contenido principal del artículo
Resumen
Detalles del artículo
La RACC aplicará la licencia internacional de atribuciones comunes creativas (Reconocimiento 4.0 Internacional: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Bajo esta licencia, se permite cualquier explotación de la obra, incluyendo la explotación con fines comerciales y la creación de obras derivadas, la distribución de las cuales también está permitida sin ninguna restricción. Esta licencia es una licencia libre según la Freedom Defined. La única condición es que siempre y en todos los casos se cite a los autores y a la fuente original de publicación (i.e., RACC). Esta licencia fue desarrollada para facilitar el acceso abierto, gratuito y libre a trabajos originales científicos y artísticos.
Cómo citar
Referencias
Beauchamp, G. K. & Mennella, A. (2011). Flavor Perception in Human Infants: Developmentand Functional Significance. Digestion, 83, 1–6.
Berridge, K.C. (2000). Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 173-198.
Blais, I., Terkel, J. & Goldblatt, A. (2006). Long-Term Impact of Early Olfactory Experience on Later Olfactory Conditioning. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 501-507.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss: Volume 1: Attachment. London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-Analysis.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent–child attachment and healthy human development. New York: Basic Books.
Cheslock, S. J., Sanders, S. K. & Spear, N. E. (2004). Learning during the newborn’s first meal; special resistance to retroactive
interference. Developmental Science, 7, 581-598.
Cheslock, S. J., Varlinskaya, E. I., Petrov, E. S. & Spear, N. E. (2000). Rapid and robust olfactory conditioning with milk before suckling experience: Promotion of nipple attachment in the newborn rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, 484-495.
Cheslock, S. J., Varlinskaya, E. I., High, J. M. & Spear, N. E. (2003). Higher order conditioning in the newborn rat; Effects of temporal disparity imply infantile encoding of simultaneous events. Infancy, 4, 157-176.
Culleré, M. E., Spear, N. E. & Molina, J. C. (2014). Prenatal ethanol increases sucrose reinforcement, an effect strengthened by postnatal association of ethanol and sucrose. Alcohol, 48, 25-33.
Faas, A. E., Spontón, E. D., Moya P. R. & Molina, J.C. (2000). Differential responsiveness to alcohol odor in human neonates: Efects of maternal consumption during gestation. Alcohol, 22, 7–17.
Fletcher, M. & Wilson, D.A. (2001). Ontogeny of odor discrimination A method to assess novel odor discrimination in neonatal rats. Physiology & Behavior, 74, 589-593.
Haberly, L. B. (1985). Neuronal Circuitry in Olfactory Cortex: Anatomy and Functional Implications. Chemical Senses, 10, 219-238.
Hofer, Myron A. (2005). The psychobiology of early attachment. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 4, 291–300.
Kamenetzky, G. V. & Mustaca, A. E. (2005). Modelos animales para el estudio del alcoholismo. Terapia Psicológica, 23, 65-72.
Janzen, M. I. D., Timmermans, P. J. A., Kruijt, J.P. & Vossen, J. M. H. (1999). Do young guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) develop an attachment to inanimate objects? Behavioural Processes, 47, 45 – 52.
Landers, M. S. & Sullivan, R. M. (2012). The Development and Neurobiology of Infant Attachment and Fear. Developmental Neuroscience, 34, 101–114.
Levine, S. (2001). Primary social relationships influence the development of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis in the rat. Physiology & Behavior, 73, 255-260.
Liem, D. G. & Mennella, J. A. (2002). Sweet and Sour Preferences During Childhood: Role of Early Experiences. Developmental Psychobiology, 41, 388-395.
Lipchock, S. V., Reed, D. R. & Mennella, J. A. (2011). The gustatory and olfactory systems during infancy: Implications for development of feeding behaviors in the high risk neonate. Clinics in Perinatology, 38, 627–641.
Lorenz, K. (1937). The Companion in the Bird's World. The Auk, 54, 245-273.
Ludington-Hoe, S. M. & Morgan, K. (2014). Infant Assessment and Reduction of Sudden Unexpected Postnatal Collapse Risk During
Skin-to-Skin Contact. Newborn & Infant Nursing Reviews, 14, 28–33.
Mair, R. G. & Gesteland, R. C. (1982). Response Properties of Mitral Cells in the Olfactory bulb of the Neonatal Rat. Neuroscience, 7, 3117-3125.
Marlier, L., Schaal, B. & Soussignan, R. (1998). Neonatal Responsiveness to the Odor of Amniotic and Lacteal Fluids: A Test of Perinatal Chemosensory Continuity. Child Development, 69, 611-623.
Mason, W. A. & Mendoza, S. P. (1998). Generic Aspects of Primate Attachments: Parents, offsprig and mates. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 765 – 778.
Mennella, J. A. (2006). Infants’ Suckling Responses to the Flavor of Alcohol in Mothers’ Milk. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 21, 581-585.
Mennella, J. A. & Beauchamp, G. K. (1991a). The Transfer of Alcohol to Human Milk: Effects on Flavor and the Infant’s Behavior. The New England Journal of Medicine, 325, 981-985.
Mennella, J. A. & Beauchamp, G. K. (1991b). Maternal Diet Alters Alters the Sensory Qualities of Human Milk and the Nursling's Behavior. Pediatrics, 88, 737-744.
Mennella, J. A. & Beauchamp, G. K. (1996). Developmental Changes in the Acceptance of Protein Hydrolysate Formula. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 17, 386-391.
Mennella, J. A. & Beauchamp, G. K. (2008). Optimizing Oral Medications for Children. Clinical Therapeutics, 30, 2120-2132.
Mennella, J. A., Jagnow, C. P. & Beauchamp, G. K. (2001). Prenatal and Postnatal Flavor Learning by Human Infants. Pediatrics, 107, 88-94.
Mennella, J. A., Johnson, A. & Beauchamp, G. K. (1995). Garlic Ingestion by Pregnant Women Alters the Odor of Amniotic Fluid. Chemical Senses, 20, 207-209.
Mennella, J. A., Spector, A. C., Reed, D. R. & Coldwell, S. E. (2013). The Bad Taste of Medicines: Overview of Basic Research on Bitter Taste. Clinical Therapeutics, 35, 1225-1246.
Miller, S. S. & Spear, N. E. (2008). Olfactory Learning in the Rat Neonate Soon After Birth. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 554-565.
Miller, S. S. & Spear, N. E. (2009). Olfactory Learning in the Rat Immediately After Birth: Unique Salience of First Odors. Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 488-504.
Miller, S. S. & Spear, N. E. (2010). Mere Odor Exposure Learning in the Rat Neonate Immediately After Birth and 1 Day Later. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 343-351.
Moriceau S., Roth T. L., Okotoghaide T. & Sullivan R. M. (2004). Corticosterone controls the developmental emergence of fear and amygdala function to predator odors in infant rat pups. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 22, 415–422.
Moriceau, S., Shionoya, K., Jakubs, K. & Sullivan R. (2009). Early-Life Stress Disrupts Attachment Learning: The Role of Amygdala Corticosterone, Locus Ceruleus Corticotropin Releasing Hormone, and Olfactory Bulb Norepinephrine. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 5745–15755.
Moriceau, S. & Sullivan, R. M. (2005). Neurobiology of Infant Attachment. Developmental Psychobiology, 47, 230–242.
Moriceau, S., Wilson, D. A., Levine, S. & Sullivan, R. M. (2006). Dual Circuitry for Odor–Shock Conditioning during Infancy: Corticosterone Switches between Fear and Attraction via Amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 6737–6748.
Morrow-Tesch, J. & McGlone, J. J. (1990). Sensory Systems and Nipple Attachment Behavior in Neonatal Pigs. Physiology & Behavior, 47, 1-4.
Nizhnikov, M.E., Petrov, E.S., Varlinskaya, E.I., Spear N.E. (2002). Newborn Rats’ First Suckling Experience: Taste Differentiation and Suckling Plasticity. Physiology & Behavior, 76, 181– 198.
Petrov, E. S., Varlinskaya, E. I. & Smotherman, W. P. (1997). The Newborn Rat Ingests Fluids through a Surrogate Nipple: A New Technique for the Study of Early Suckling Behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 62, 1155–1158.
Petrov, E.S., Varlinskaya, E.I. & Spear, N.E. (2003). The surrogate nipple technique in the rat provides a useful animal model of
suckling in bottle-feeding circumstances: reply to Blass (2002). Physiology & Behavior, 78, 813– 817.
Phillips, R. (2013). The Sacred Hour: Uninterrupted Skin-to-Skin Contact Immediately After Birth. Newborn & Infant Nursing Reviews, 13, 67–72.
Porter, R. H. & Winberg, J. (1999). Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 439-449.
Raineki, C., Moriceau, S. & Sullivan, R. M. (2010). Developing a Neurobehavioral Animal Model of Infant Attachment to an Abusive Caregiver. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 1137-1145.
Redican, W. K. & Kaplan J. N. (1978). Effects of Synthetic Odors on Filial Attachment in Infant Squirrel Monkeys. Physiology & Behavior, 20, 79-85.
Reis, A. R., Azevedo, M. S., Souza, M. A., Lutz, M. L., Alves, M. B., Izquierdo, I., ...Lucion, A. B. (2014). Neonatal handling alters the structure of maternal behavior and affects mother–pup bonding. Behavioural Brain Research, 265, 216–228.
Roth, T. L. & Sullivan, R. M. (2003). Consolidation and expression of a shock-induced odor preference in rat pups is facilitated by opioids. Physiology & Behavior, 78, 135– 142.
Schaal, B., Marlier, L. & Soussignan, R. (1998). Olfactory Function in the Human Fetus: Evidence From Selective Responsiveness to the Odor of Amniotic Fluid. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 1438-1449.
Schaal, B., Marlier, L. & Soussignan, R. (2000). Human Foetuses Learn Odours from their Pregnant Mother’s Diet. Chemical Senses, 25, 729–737.
Scott, J. A., Chih, T. Y. & Oddy, W. H. (2012). Food Variety at 2 Years of Age is Related to Duration of Breastfeeding. Nutrients, 4, 1464-1474.
Suárez, A., Mustaca, A, Pautassi, R. y Kamenetzky, G. (2014). Ontogeny of consummatory successive negative contrast in rats. Developmental Psychobiology.
Sullivan R. M. & Holman, P. J. (2010). Transitions in sensitive period attachment learning in infancy: The role of corticosterone. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 835-844.
Sullivan, R. M., Landers, M., Yeaman, B. & Wilson, D. A. (2000). Good memory of bad events in infancy. Nature, 407, 38-39.
Sullivan, R. M., Stackenwalt, G., Nasr, F., Lemon, C. & Wilson, D. A. (2000). Association of an Odor with Activation of Olfactory Bulb Noradrenergic Î-Receptors or Locus Coeruleus Stimulation is Sufficient to Produce Learned Approach Responses to that Odor in
Neonatal Rats. Developmental Neuroscience, 114, 957-962.
Sullivan, R. M., Taborsky-Barba, S., Mendoza, R., Itano, A., Leon, M., Cotman, C. W., Payne, T. F. & Lott, I. (1991). Olfactory Classical Conditioning in Neonates. Pediatrics, 4, 511–518.
Sullivan, R. M., Wilson, D. A., Lemon, C. & Gerhardt, G. A. (1994). Bilateral 6-OHDA lesions of the locus coeruleus impair associative olfatory learning in newborn rats. Brain Research, 643, 306-309.
Sullivan, R. M., Wilson, D. A. & Leon, M. (1989). Norepinephrine and Learning-Induced Plasticity in Infant Rat Olfactory System. The Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 3998-4008.
Sullivan, R. M., Zyzak, D. R., Skierkowski, P. & Wilson, D. A. (1992). The role of olfactory bulb norepinephrine in early olfactory learning. Developmental Brain Research, 70, 279 -282.
Upton, K. J. & Sullivan, R. M. (2010). Defining Age Limits of the Sensitive Period for Attachment. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 453-464.
Varendi H., Porter R. H. & Winberg J. (1996). Attractiveness of amniotic fluid odor: evidence of prenatal olfactory learning? Acta Pediatrica, 85, 1223–1227.